اثر بارِ شناختی و بازة زمانی بر ادراک زمان
الموضوعات :علی شهابی فر 1 , سید مهدی حسینی فرد 2 , عطاالله موحدی نیا 3
1 - دانشگلاه آزاد اسلامی، واحد زرند
2 - دانشگاه علوم پزشکی کرمان
3 - دانشگاه آزاد اسلامی زرند
الکلمات المفتاحية: ادراک زمان بازتولید زمان بارِ شناختیبازة زمانی ,
ملخص المقالة :
هدف از پژوهش حاضر بررسی اثر بارِ شناختی و بازة زمانی و تعامل این دو متغیر بر ادراک زمان به روش شبه آزمایشی است. جامعه آماری این پژوهش، دانش آموزان، دانشجویان، و کارکنان سازمان های شهر کرمان در سال 1397 بودند که از میان آنها 300 نفر (150 مرد و 150 زن) به صورت در دسترس انتخاب شدند و همه آنها تکلیف بازتولید زمان (TP) را انجام دادند. نتایج نشان دادند که هم اثر اصلی بارِ شناختی و بازة زمانی و هم اثر تعاملی این دو بر ادراک زمان معنا دار است (01/0>p). بارِ شناختی و افزایش بازة زمانی بر ادراک زمان به صورت افزایش میزان خطای بازتولید زمان اثر دارند. لازم به ذکر است که بیشترین تفاوت در خطای بازتولید زمان بین بازه های 2 و 8 ثانیه به دست آمد (001/0=p).
اختياري، حامد؛ جنتي، علی؛ پرهيزگار، سید احسان؛ بهزادي، آرین و مكري، آذرخش (1382). ادراك زمان و روش¬هاي ارزيابي آن: يك مطالعه مقدماتي براي آزمودني فارسي زبان. فصلنامه تازه¬هاي علوم شناختي، 5، 49-36.
جوادی، حسین و جوادی، افسانه (1387). فیزیک از آغاز تا امروز. تهران: اندرز.
دلاور، علی (1391). مبانی نظری و عملی پژوهش در علوم انسانی و اجتماعی. تهران: رشد.
شهابی¬فر، علی (1393). مقایسه ادراک زمان در معتادین به مواد مخدر و افراد عادی. پایان¬نامه کارشناسی ارشد، دانشگاه آزاد اسلامی واحد زرند.
نظري، محمدعلی؛ راستکارهاشمی، نسرین؛ دمپا، سمیرا؛ سعیدي¬دهاقانی، سمیه؛ سلطان¬لو، مجتبی و میرلو، محمدمهدي (1392). نقش جنسیت، والانس و برانگیختگی لغات فارسی داراي بار هیجانی بر ادراك زمان. دوفصلنامه علمی پژوهشی شناخت اجتماعی، 4، 62-73.
نوروزی، سمیه؛ نظری، محمدعلی؛ جهان، علی؛ و اسماعیل¬پور، خلیل. تفاوت¬های جنسیتی در ادراک زمان در رابطه با تعامل عدد و زمان. روانشناسی و روان¬پزشکی شناخت، ۱(۱)، ۲۲-10. Ahn, H. K., Liu, M. W., & Soman, D. (2006). Memory for time: A cognitive model of retrospective duration and sequence judgments. Acta Neurobiologiae Experimentalis, 64(3), 319–328.
Allman, M. J., Yin, B., & Meck, W. H. (2014). Time in the psycho-pathological mind. In D. Lloyd & V. Arstila (Eds.), Subjective Time: The Philosophy, Psychology, and Neuroscience of Temporality (pp. 637-654), Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Block, F., & Gellersen, H. (2010). The impact of cognitive load on the perception of time, Proceedings: NordiCHI,10, 16-20.
Block, R. A., & Zakay, D. (2008). Timing and remembering the past, the present, and the future. In S. Grondin (Ed.), Psychology of Time (pp. 367-394),. Bingley, England: Emerald.
Block, R. A., & Gruber, R. P. (2014). Time perception, attention, and memory: A selective review. Acta Psychologica, 149, 129-133.
Block, R. A., & Zakay, D. (1997). Prospective and retrospective duration judgments: A meta-analytic review. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 4(2), 184-197.
Block, R. A., & Zakay, D. (2006). Prospective remembering involves time estimation and memory processes. In J. Glicksohn & M. S. Myslobodsky (Eds.), Timing the future: The case for a time-based prospective memory (pp. 25-49). London: World Scientific.
Block, R. A., George, E. J., & Reed, M. A. (1980). A watched pot sometimes boils: A study of duration experience. Acta Psychologica, 46, 81-94.
Block, R. A., Hancock, P. A., & Zakay, D. (2010). How cognitive load affects duration judgments: A meta-analytic review. Acta Psychologica, 134, 330-343.
Block, R. A., & Reed, M. A. (1978). Remembered duration: Evidence for a contextual-change hypothesis. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 4, 656-665.
Brown, S. W. (2008). Time and attention: Review of the literature. In S. Grondin (Ed.), Psychology of time (pp. 111-138), Bingley, England: Emerald.
Brown, S. W. (1985). Time perception and attention: The effects of prospective versus retrospective paradigms and task demands on perceived duration. Perception & Psychophysics, 38, 115-124.
Brown, S. W. (1997). Attentional resources in timing: Interference effects in concurrent temporal and nontemporal working memory tasks. Perception & Psychophysics, 59, 1118-1140.
Buhusi, C. V., & Meck, W. H. (2005). What makes us tick? Functional and neural mechanisms of interval timing. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 6(10), 755-765.
Callender, C. (2010). Is time an illusion? The concepts of time and change may emerge from a universe that, at root, is utterly static. Scientific American, 302(6), 58-65.
Conson, M., Cinque, F., Barbarulo, A. M., & Trojano, L. (2008). A common processing system for duration, order and spatial information: Evidence from a time estimation task. Experimental Brain Research, 187, 267-274.
Desai, V. R. (2007). The effects of aging and brain damage on time perception. CUREJ: College Undergraduate Research Electronic Journal, University of Pennsylvania, http://repository.upenn.edu/curej/60.
Dormal, V., Seron, X., & Pesenti, M. (2006). Numerosity-duration interference: A stroop experiment. Acta Psychologica, 121, 109-124.
Duzcu, H., & Hohenberger, A. (2014). Prospective duration judgments: The role of temporality and executive demands of the concurrent task. Acta Psychologica, 147, 34-41.
Ferreira, V. F. M., Paiva, G. P., Prando, N., Graça, C. R., & Kouyoumdjian, J. A. (2016). Time perception and age. Arquivos de Neuro-Psiquiatria, 74(4), 299-302.
Ginns, P., & Leppink, J. (2019). Special issue on cognitive load theory: Editorial. Psychological Review, 31, 255-259.
Glicksohn, J. (2001). Temporal cognition and the phenomenology of time: A multiplicative function for apparent duration. Consciousness and Cognition, 10, 1-25.
Goldstone, S., Lhamon, W. T., & Sechzer, J. (1978). Light intensity and judged duration. Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society, 12, 83-84.
Grondin, S. (2001). From physical time to the first and second moments of psychological time. Psychological Bulletin, 127(1), 22-44.
Grondin, S. (2010). Timing and time perception: A review of recent behavioral and neuroscience findings and theoretical directions. Attention, Perception & Psychophysics, 75, 561-582.
Hamamouche, K., Keefe, M., Jordan, K. E., & Cordes, S. (2018). Cognitive load affects numerical and temporal judgments in distinct ways. Frontiers in Psychology, 9, 1783.
Hancock, P. A., & Szalma, J. L. (Eds.) (2008). Performance under stress. Aldershot, England: Ashgate.
Hurewitz, F., Gelman, R., & Schnitzer, B. (2006). Sometimes area counts more than number. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 103, 19599-19604.
Kalyuga, S. (2009). Cognitive load factors in instructional design for advanced learners. New York, NY: Nova Science Publishers, Inc.
Khan, A., Sharma, N. K., & Dixit, S. (2006). Effect of cognitive load and paradigm on time perception. Indian Academy of Applied Psychology, 32(1), 37-42.
Lloyd, D., & Arstila, V. (2014). Subjective time: Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Macar, F., & Vidal, F. (2009). Timing processes: An outline of behavioural and neural indices not systematically considered in timing models. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology, 63, 227-239.
McClain, L. (1983). Interval estimation: Effect of processing demands on prospective and retrospective reports. Perception & Psychophysics, 34, 185-189.
Pinel, P., Piazza, M., Le Bihan, D., & Dehaene, S. (2004). Distributed and overlapping cerebral representations of number, size, and luminance during comparative judgments. Neuron, 41, 983-993.
Podvigina, D. N., & Lyakhovetskii, V. A. (2011). Characteristics of the perception of short time intervals. Neuroscience and Behavioral Physiology, 41, 9, 936-941.
Quartier, V., Zimmermann, G., & Nashat, S. (2010). sense of time in children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD). Swiss Journal of Psychology, 69, 7-14.
Rovee-Collier, C. (1995). Time window in cognitive development. Developmental Psychology, 31, 147-169.
Sari, F. B. (2015). Effects of kind and amount of cognitive load and duration on prospective time estimation. Master’s Thesis, Middle East Technical University, Ankara, Turkey.
Shahabifar A., & Movahedinia, A. (2016). Comparing time perception among morphine- derived drugs addicts and controls. Addict Health, 8(1), 32-40.
Sucala, M., Scheckner, B., & David, D. (2010). Psychological time: Interval length judgments and subjective passage of time judgments. Current Psychology Letters [Online], 26(2).
Sweller, J. (2010). Element interactivity and intrinsic, extraneous and germane cognitive load. Educational Psychology Review, 22, 123–138.
Usal, K. A., & Hohenberger, A. (2017). The effects of social task setting on time perception. In G. Gunzelmann, A. Howes, T. Tenbrink, & E. J. Davelaar (Eds.), Proceedings of the 39th Annual Meeting of the Cognitive Science Society (pp. 3423-3428). London: United Kingdom.
Van Rijn, H., & Taatgen, N. A. (2008). Timing of multiple overlapping time intervals: How many clocks do we have? Acta Psychologica, 129(3), 365-375.
Wittmann, M. (2013). The inner sense of time: How the brain creates a representation of duration. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 14, 217-223.
Xuan, B., Zhang, D., He, S., & Chen, X. (2007). Larger stimuli are judged to last longer. Journal of Vision, 7, 1-5.
Zakay, D. (1993). Time estimation methods, do they influence prospective duration estimates? Perception, 22, 91-101.
Zakay, D., & Block, R. A. (1995). An attentional-gate model of prospective time estimation. In M. Richelle, V. De Keyser, G. d'Ydewalle, & A. Ndierendonck (Eds.), Time and the dynamic control of behavior (pp. 167-178). Liège, Belgium: Universite de Liege.
Zakay, D., & Block, R. A. (1996). The role of attention in time estimation processes. In M. A. Pastor (Ed.), Time, internal clocks, and movement (pp. 143-164). Amsterdam: Elsevier.
Zakay, D. & Block, R. A. (2004). Prospective and retrospective duration judgments: An executive-control process. Acta Neurobiologiae Experi-mentalis, 64, 319-328.
Zélanti P. S., & Droit-Volet S. (2011). Cognitive abilities explaining age related changes in time perception of short and long durations. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 109(2), 143-157.